| Home | E-Submission | Sitemap | Editorial Office |  
top_img
Audiology and Speech Research > Volume 21(1); 2025 > Article
Puttanna, Harbi, Fernandes, and Dsilva: Understanding Discourse Genre and Its Effects in Persons with Fluent Aphasia: A Preliminary Investigation

Abstract

Purpose

Discourse analysis is an important component of aphasia assessment because it can provide insight into functional communication abilities of the participants. Understanding the essence of discourse becomes pivotal, as it is the most rudimentary way of communication in individuals’ daily routine. Hence, the present study aimed to explore the superiority of different discourse elicitation tasks in persons with fluent aphasia (PwFA).

Methods

A total of six PwFA were recruited for the study. These participants were assessed for their discourse abilities using three tasks: picture description, conversation, and narration. The participants' discourse samples were analysed using the discourse analysis scale. In all these tasks, the propositional and non-propositional aspects of communication were evaluated. After recording the responses, the scores of each task (picture description, conversation, and narration) were consolidated to compute the discourse quotient.

Results

Quantitative analysis revealed significant differences across general conversation compared to narration and picture description tasks. Out of these three tasks, general conversation is outperformed the other two tasks. Further, the individual case-wise analysis showed similar pattern of results.

Conclusion

The findings of this study hold significant implications for both clinical practice and research. Understanding the potency and setbacks of different discourse genres aids speech language pathologist to analyse the discourse elicited from different genres using different cutoff values. Despite the positive findings of the current study the findings of the study should be interpreted meticulously due to the small sample size and the limited ecological validity of the study.

INTRODUCTION

Aphasia is a debilitated condition that hampers the patients’ communication. This condition can be due to stroke, and approximately 30% of stroke survivors may be prone to develop aphasia following a stroke (Sekine et al., 2013). A large pool of linguistic research is conducted to understand language performance in persons with aphasia (PWAs), which may aid speech-language pathologists (SLPs)/researchers to get better insight during their rehabilitation. Previous linguistic aphasia research studies mainly focused on processing language components at the word or phrase level in the absence of the context (Armstrong, 2000). However, in recent times, there has been a paradigm shift, and researchers have appraised language at the sentence level (Bryant et al., 2016). Also, they aim to understand the cohesion and coherence of language via discourse tasks in stroke survivors with language impairment to improve aphasia assessment in clinical practices (Richardson & Dalton, 2020). Richardson & Dalton (2020) conducted a study focusing on creating a checklist for analysing the main concepts in narratives elicited by pictures prompts involving a cat in a tree and a person refusing an umbrella elicited narrative tasks in neurotypical healthy adults across age groups 20~39, 40~59, 60~79, and >80. The findings of the study evinced the normative range for each age group for each picture individually. These results aid SLPs and researchers in ascertaining the extent of deviation in discourse skills from normalcy in PWAs.
Discourse can be elucidated as one of the units of language that goes beyond the sentence level. This encompasses the use of various linguistic structures such as phonology, lexicon, syntax, semantics, and discourse markers. These linguistic structures aid individuals to convey appropriate meaning of the context (Cavanaugh & Haley, 2020; Stemmer, 1999; Wright, 2011). Discourse-based studies in PWAs were based on structural and functional aspects of discourse per se. Former aspects of discourse were elucidated as it is solely based on the measures of syntax and lexical production during picture description or while describing personal narrative. The latter discourse measure was focused on how meaning is organized at the pragmatic interactive level (Armstrong, 2000; Pritchard et al., 2017).
One of the widely exploited methods to obtain representative speech samples in PWAs is the picture description task which is frequently included in language standardized tests (Vandenborre et al., 2018). It offers several advantages over conversational analysis, such as obtaining an object reference to the conversational topic (Fergadiotis et al., 2011), and memory and sustained attentional demands are minimized (McNeil et al., 2004). In comparison to picture description, storytelling is deemed to increase lexical productivity and propositional density, which gives rise to what we might call expository or descriptive discourse (Alyahya et al., 2022; Stark, 2019). Despite storytelling being the pivotal task to measure discourse, this task could be challenging in PWAs owing to the involvement of memory and planning per se. In this line, a study by Stark et al.(2023) ascertained PWAs had a reduced type-token ratio and reduced words per minute during narrative tasks relative to other forms of discourse genre.

Discourse in aphasia

Discourse varies with different elicitation methods (Doyle et al., 1995; Roberts & Wertz, 1989). Roberts & Wertz(1989) aimed to compare the structured and unstructured language tasks in PWAs. A total of 20 PWAs were enrolled in the study and their language sample were audio-video recorded. In specific to the elicitation of language samples, conversation tasks and verbal descriptions of the object were used and the same tasks were compared across 1, 3, 6, 9, and 12 months post-stroke onset. The study findings showed there is a difference between the two different elicitation methods.
Understanding the relationship between different discourse elicitation methods and their influence on language is deemed pivotal. These aid researchers/SLPs in designing the effective treatment plan for PWAs (Shadden et al., 1991). In a similar line, a study by McCullough et al.(2017) aimed to compare the language demand imposed for structured and conversation discourse tasks in PWAs. Ten PWAs inclusive of both fluent and non-fluent aphasia were recruited for the study and discourse was analysed using the correct information unit. The findings of the study showed that there is a significant difference in discourse elicitation tasks (structured versus conversation tasks).
In addition, McCullough et al.(2017) ascertained fluent aphasia outperformed non-fluent aphasia in the conversation task, but no significant difference was evinced between fluent and non-fluent aphasia for the structured task. The above findings shed light on differences in the task demand and its influence on the language. This finding acknowledges the fact that each discourse elicitation method yields distinct discourse abilities. Thus, there arises a need to conduct research in this line to a large extent.
Discourse analysis is an important component of aphasia assessment because it can provide insight into functional communication abilities. The purpose of the current study is to explore the possible differences in discourse-level communication in persons with fluent aphasia (PwFA). Discourse analysis provides a rich data source to gauge the linguistic content/structure of language use in a more naturalistic way (Hazamy & Obermeyer, 2020).
Previous studies on fluent aphasia posit that lexical and semantic impairment is predominantly noted in PwFA (Edwards, 2005). Also, researchers noted they manifest a larger amount of grammatical impairment during discourse production (Bastiaanse et al., 1996). This necessitates the researcher to understand the potency of different discourse genres specific to fluent aphasia. Further, Andreetta et al.(2012) compared the cohesion of anomic aphasia with their age-matched control group. The findings of the study portrayed a large number of cohesion errors during a narrative discourse task in persons with anomic aphasia compared with the age-matched control group.
On similar lines, Manning & Franklin(2016) compared the narrative discourse abilities in persons with fluent and nonfluent aphasia. The findings of the study unveil that there is a difference in macro and microstructural aspects of discourse. In specific, non-fluent aphasia are more prone to have higher rates of subject/object omissions and article errors than fluent aphasia. Fluent aphasia produced errors concerning information being presented in illogical order with more pronoun difficulty compared to non-fluent aphasia. All these findings alluded to earlier were unable to show statistical differences. The study results shed light on the need for establishing comparative norms across different types of aphasia. Ascertaining the commonalities and differences among the variant types of aphasia is paramount, owing to the identification and selection of appropriate assessment and intervention approaches in PWAs.

Need for the study

Previous literature on discourse-based seems inconclusive, owing to different methods employed to analyse the discourse, single case description, and distinct linguistic profile (Zhang et al., 2021; Marangolo et al., 2014). Discourse samples can be analyzed using different elicitation procedures. Out of the discrete discourse elicitation procedures, the picture description task stands out as a common and frequently used procedure. Storytelling activities are carried out in a way by narrating the individual’s personal experience or by narrating familiar stories. This activity is employed more in recent studies than in regular clinical practice (Andreetta et al., 2012). Storytelling/narration is also known to be a more naturalist way of eliciting discourse. Personal narratives result in inconsistencies both across and within participants, due to heterogeneity of experience of each individual. Procedural description (e.g., preparation of oats/coffee) involves a natural mode of connected speech. However, in most of the aphasia literature or clinical practice, the procedural description task was under-utilized.
Furthermore, the difference across the distinct genres of discourse is more explicitly elucidated in the theoretical framework of discourse production (Sherratt, 2007). Sherratt(2007) postulates the fact that discourse takes place at different stages, and it varies with different genres. The literature on discourse genre tries to understand the potent of each genre in isolation, only a handful of studies tried to compare the superiority of genres in PWAs (e.g., Stark, 2019; Armstrong et al., 2011). In addition, most of the studies evinced paradoxical findings in establishing the relationship between discourse and language impairment. (e.g., Olness & Ulatowska, 2011). In addition, understanding the insight of discourse becomes pivotal, as it is the most rudimentary way in which individuals have their conversation in their daily day-to-day routines. (Andreetta et al., 2012). Thus, to bridge these alluded research gaps the present study aimed to uncover the superiority of different discourse elicitation tasks in PwFA.

Research question

Does the performance of discourse genres differ (descriptive, narration, and conversation) in PwFA?

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Participants

In the current study, a total of six PwFA were recruited, who were recruited from in and around Mangalore district, India via a convenient sampling (Table 1). All the participants gave their consent to be part of the study. The study received ethical clearance from Father Muller Charitable Institutions (FMCI) (FMMCIEC/CCM/611/2022).
The current study followed stringent criteria to recruit the participants. 1) The PwFA should have a minimum of 6 months post-stroke onset. 2) PwFA should have at least cleared their matriculation with fairly good reading skills in Kannada. 3) PwFA should possess normal hearing/good vision/corrected vision. 4) PwFA should have a left middle cerebral artery lesion. 5) PwFA should be free from cognitive impairment were ruled out using Montreal Cognitive Assessment (Nasreddine et al., 2005). And 6) PwFA with psychiatric issues were excluded.

Materials

In the current study pictures of ‘broken windows’, ‘cat rescue’, and ‘refused umbrella’ were used for the picture description task. Western Aphasia Battery in Kannada was used to gauge the Aphasia Quotient and type of aphasia. Audio-video recorder (QIWA® Wireless HD 960P; AUSHA overseas, Bangalore, India) was used to record the discourse sample.

Procedure

PWAs were assessed across three tasks namely; picture description, conversation, and narration. In the picture description, the PwFAs were asked to describe ‘broken windows’, ‘refuse umbrella’, and ‘cat in the tree’ pictures (Nicholas & Brookshire, 1993) (Appendix 1). The description of pictures was solely based on PWA’s discretion. In this task, verbal prompts or cueing were not encouraged. In conversation task, PwFAs, were probed questions about their hobbies/daily routine/current affairs. Here, PWAs were given adequate time (60~80 seconds) to respond to each question. If they were unable to maintain the conversation then the researcher tried having a conversation with a different topic. If still PwFA fails then the researcher moves to the next task without giving any feedback. In the narration task, PwFAs were asked to narrate the events of their interest namely, stories, explaining their job profile, and explaining their favorite book/books they have read. All these discourse samples were audio-video recorded and verbatim of each sample were transcribed and later used for discourse analysis.
Computing discourse via the aforementioned task was further subjected to analysis using the discourse analysis scale (Hema & Shyamala, 2008) (Appendix 2). In all these tasks, the propositional and non-propositional aspects of communication were evaluated. Discourse structure, communication intent, topic management, and so on are some of the propositional aspects of communication. Repair strategies, revision behaviour, turn-taking, etc., are some of the non-propositional aspects of communication. After recording the responses, the scores of each task (picture description, conversation and narration) were consolidated to compute the discourse quotient (DQ).
For the conversation analysis there are total of ‘38’ parameters, inclusive of both propositional and nonpropositional aspects of discourse. Each parameters receive maximum score of ‘2’, thus maximum score participants can obtained from this task is ‘76’. Subsequently, for the narration task, there are total of ‘26’ parameters, hence, the participants can score maximum of ‘52’ in this task. In picture description task, there were total of ‘28’ parameters, hence the participants can score maximum of ‘56’ in this task. For instance, if a PWA scores 48/56 in the picture description task, then DQ for the picture description task would be 61.53% (48/56 × 100 = 85.71%).

Scoring

Each discourse sample across the tasks was scored based on the discourse analysis scale (Hema & Shyamala, 2008). It is a three-point rating scale, 0 is poor, 1 is fair, and 2 is good. Tasks alluded to earlier were scored based on a three-point rating scale (Appendix 1B).

Data and statistical analysis

The computed data was fed into SPSS software for statistical analysis analysis version 18 (IBM, Chicago, IL, USA). The Friedman test was used to compare the statistical differences across different discourse genres, and further Wilcoxon signed ranked test was used to compare the pair wise differences among the discourse tasks. Data was also subjected for reliability assessment using intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) to obtain inter-rater and intrarater reliability for the discourse tasks (narration, picture description, and conversation).

Reliability measures

5% of the discourse sample was given to the ‘2’ experienced speech language pathologists for assessing the effectiveness of discourse quotient. The findings revealed ICC of 0.85 (95% confidence interval [CI], 0.65 to 0.95), indicating excellent reliability. Further, intra-rated reliability revealed an ICC of 0.90 (95% CI, 0.70 to 0.98), indicating higher consistency in the rater’s assessments.

RESULTS

The data was fed into SPSS software version 18 (IBM) to compute descriptive and quantitative analysis. The mean and standard deviation for the general conversation, narration, and picture description tasks were computed. Pronounced scores were evinced for general conversation compared to narration and picture description tasks. On comparing narration and picture description, narration scores were higher compared to picture description (Figure 1). Subsequent to group analysis, the data was analyzed individually via qualitative description. This analysis aimed to understand the subtle changes within each participant.
The data were subjected to statistical analysis using the Friedman test, this test was applied to gauge the overall difference across the tasks. The findings of the study evinced significant differences across the general conversation, narration, and picture description tasks (χ2(2) = 9.47; p = 0.09). Further, the Wilcoxon sign ranked test was applied to carry out a pair-wise comparison. The results of this test revealed a significant difference across general conversation versus narration, and general conversation versus picture description. However, the researcher ascertained no significant difference while comparing picture description and narration tasks (Table 2).

Individual data analysis

Individual analysis revealed that the partcipant (P)1, P2, and P6 scores for general conversation were higher followed by narration and picture description tasks (Figure 2). The participants P4 and P5 evinced pronounced scores for general conversation, but the equivalent scores were noted for narration and picture description tasks. Participant P3 scores were higher in conversation; however, the robust score was noted for picture description than narration. Overall, all these participants outperformed in the conversation task compared to the narration and picture description task.

DISCUSSIONS

The study aims to shed light on the superiority of three distinct discourse tasks (conversation, narration, and picture description) in PwFA. The findings of the study partially supported the hypothesis of the study with no significant difference between narration and picture description tasks. On the other hand, the findings of the study rejected the hypothesis, where significant difference was manifested across conversation versus narration tasks; and conversation versus picture description tasks.
The experimental paradigm conceived in the study seems to be novel. The literature in this line of research is scanty. Hence, the current study findings are discussed merely based on discourse-based studies in PWAs. Conversation tasks manifested more robustness than other tasks (narration and picture description) which could be asserted to lack of density of language requirement (Stark, 2019). That is, during conversation PWAs respond merely to the questions posed by the researcher. Hence, they would be able to use only limited vocabulary, limited phrases, and reduced nouns/verbs compared to narration tasks. Owing, to which conversation tasks impose lesser taxes on the linguistic abilities of PWAs, resulting in better discourse scores relative to other tasks.
Conversation tasks utilize lesser processes of memory relative to narrative tasks and picture description tasks. In narrative tasks, there is the involvement of long-term and working memory to a larger extent. In addition, executive functioning skills such as planning and programming would play a pivotal role in narration compared to conversation tasks. Owing to the aforementioned elucidations, it is perceptible that linguistic complexity varies with conversation, narration, and picture description tasks (Stark, 2019). This might enable PWAs to perform better at conversation tasks than narration and picture description tasks.
According to cognitive load theory Sweller et al.(2011) cognitive load varies across tasks like conversation, narration, and picture description. Conversation usually entail processing information in real time with a focus on interaction. Narration task requires individuals to construct and recall events in a coherent fashion, often triggering working memory. Picture description task may require recalling details from long-term memory and organizing those into speech, this could be more demanding than simple exchange of information in conversation.
Robust performance in conversation tasks is also in accordance with the studies (Beeke et al., 2003 & 2007). Beeke et al.(2003 & 2007) have also manifested differences in the use of particular grammatical forms during monologue discourse and conversation. Also, they asserted the fact that PWAs use more complete grammatical forms during monologue discourse than conversation. Conversation tasks may aid PWAs to have assistance from a partner and enable participants to get contextual cues for effective communication. The priming effect can manifest during the conversation which in turn, may excrete lesser linguistic demand than other discourse-based tasks (Armstrong et al., 2011). Moreover, in the current study majority of the participants recruited exhibited anomic aphasia, as this type of aphasia generally performs well in the context of cues (Abhishek et al., 2018). Thus, when a conversation task is rendered to persons with anomic aphasia this might have facilitated contextual cues. Owing to this the superiority in performance of conversation tasks can be noted compared to narration and picture description tasks.
On similar lines, Bryant et al.(2016) aimed to investigate the discourse abilities of PWAs with their age-matched control group. In this study, authors used picture description tasks and sequence pictures to assess the discourse abilities. The findings of the study displayed the difference in discourse skill across picture description and sequence picture tasks. The sequence picture task resulted in more words per minute, whereas the picture description task elicited denser language with a pronounced noun-verb ratio. These results showed that discourse varies across the tasks and the type of stimuli presented. Also, the authors concluded that the picture description task is more suitable for moderate to severe forms of aphasia, and also for individuals who heavily rely on visual modality for lexical retrieval.
On the other hand, the current study findings are contraindicated with few other studies (e.g., Beeke et al., 2003; Heeschen & Schegloff, 2003). They asserted the fact that PWAs tend to produce more language with greater linguistic load during monologues than dialogues.
Further, the data was analysed individually and authors noted different trends of results. Wherein, participants P1, P2, and P6 evinced increased scores for conversation tasks followed by narration and picture description tasks. This could be alluded to earlier elucidation of how conversation yielded better discourse scores compared to the others.
Participants P4 and P5 also discerned higher scores in conversation, but scores for narration and picture description remain equipotent. This could be due to participants’ word retrieval deficits, lexical deficits, and syntactic deficits; these would have hampered their performance in specific narration and picture description tasks rather than conversation tasks. That is, narration and picture description are deemed to be constraint-based tasks, where PwFA should be able to use words and sentences specific to the topic/pictures. Thus, discourse scores for narration and picture description are impoverished. Participant P3 showed an atypical pattern compared to other participants in the study, i.e., P3 showed higher scores in conversation followed by picture description and narration tasks. This finding can be explained based on the visual cues that the participant is exposed to his lexicon activation is confounded to that particular picture/context alone without activation of neighbourhood lexicons. Thus, performance may be ameliorated by the usage of correct words and sentences. In addition, it can be posited that P3 might have better lexical retrieval in the presence of visual cues. P3 result findings should be interpreted with caution owing to anecdotal reports.
Discourse analysis is essential to the assessment of aphasia due to its ability to illuminate functional communication abilities in PWAs. The findings of this study suggest conversation scores outperformed narration and picture description tasks. The study findings would serve as important insight for both clinical and research aspects in understanding the potency and setbacks of different discourse genres. The different tasks used to assess discourse in PWAs, ascertained about the complexity of the task. This information aids SLPs to choose the appropriate discourse task based on the severity of aphasia. The study also highlights the importance of using multiple discourse tasks in order to gain a comprehensive insight on the discourse skills of the participants. Findings of the study highlight the fact that different discourse task should poses different cutoff points/different analytical approach for accurate measurement of discourse skills per se in PWAs.
Also, this study serves to be a preliminary study, which may pave way for the future studies to be conducted in a more extended and elaborated manner. Despite the robust scores for conversation tasks than other discourse genres in PwFA, the findings of the study should be interpreted meticulously owing to the small sample size; and due to usage of a qualitative scale to appraise discourse skills. Furthermore, selecting suitable discourse elicitation tasks specific to PwFA is deemed paramount, when identifying the generalization of impaired-based training to functional skills per se.
Despite the study findings that shed light on differences among different discourse genres. The current study poses limitations concerning sample size. The study design restricted the preliminary assessment merely to identify the type of aphasia and word retrieval impairment. However, it is also important to assess working memory in PwFA, owing to its reciprocal relationship with discourse abilities.

Notes

Ethical Statement

The study received ethical clearance from Father Muller Charitable Institutions (FMCI) (FMMCIEC/CCM/611/2022).

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

There are no conflict interests.

Funding

None.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: Deepak Puttanna and Mohammad Alharbi. Formal analysis: Deepak Putanna, Frannie Lamita Fernandes, and Yorick Smith Dsliva. Data collection: Frannie Lamita Fernandes and Yorick Smith Dsilva. Approval of final manuscript: all authors.

Acknowledgments

Authors like to thank Father Muller College, Deparment of Speech Language Pathology, Mangalore, India. Also, authors like to thank all the participants participated in the study.

Figure 1.
Mean and standard deviation scores for picture description, narration, and conversation tasks.
asr-240166f1.jpg
Figure 2.
Performance o f each participant across three different discourse tasks. P: participant.
asr-240166f2.jpg
Table 1.
Demographic details of persons with aphasia recruited in the study
Participant Age/sex SPO Diagnosis Aphasia quotient Education MoCA
P1 43 years/M 15 months Conduction aphasia 65.2 Matriculation 26
P2 57 years/M 11 months Anomic 79.3 Undergraduate 28
P3 63 years/M 17 months Anomic 81.5 Matriculation 27
P4 56 years/M 4 months Anomic 81.5 Postgraduate 28
P5 52 years/F 12 months Anomic 73.5 Matriculation 26
P6 79 years/M 3 months Anomic 73.5 Matriculation 26

SPO: stroke post onset, MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment, M: male, F: female

Table 2.
Pair-wise comparison of three discourse tasks in PwFA
Narration-general conversation Picture description- general conversation Picture description- narration
/z/ value 2.20 2.20 1.08
p-value 0.02* 0.02* 0.27

PwFA: persons with fluent aphasia.

* Significant difference at p < 0.05

REFERENCES

Abhishek, B. P., Akhilandeshwari, S., & Devika, V. (2018). Contextual cueing for retrieval of nouns and verbs in persons with anomic aphasia. Research & Reviews: Journal of Medical Science and Technology, 7(2), 1-5.

Alyahya, R. S., Conroy, P., Halai, A. D., & Lambon Ralph, M. A. (2022). An efficient, accurate and clinically-applicable index of content word fluency in aphasia. Aphasiology, 36(8), 921-939.
crossref pmid
Andreetta, S., Cantagallo, A., & Marini, A. (2012). Narrative discourse in anomic aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 50(8), 1787-1793.
crossref pmid
Armstrong, E. (2000). Aphasic discourse analysis: The story so far. Aphasiology, 14(9), 875-892.
crossref
Armstrong, E., Ciccone, N., Godecke, E., & Kok, B. (2011). Monologues and dialogues in aphasia: Some initial comparisons. Aphasiology, 25(11), 1347-1371.
crossref
Bastiaanse, R., Edwards, S., & Kiss, K. (1996). Fluent aphasia in three languages: Aspects of spontaneous speech. Aphasiology, 10(6), 561-575.
crossref
Beeke, S., Wilkinson, R., & Maxim, J. (2003). Exploring aphasic grammar 2: Do language testing and conversation tell a similar story? Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 17(2), 109-134.
crossref
Beeke, S., Wilkinson, R., & Maxim, J. (2007). Grammar without sentence structure: A conversation analytic investigation of agrammatism. Aphasiology, 21(3-4), 256-282.
crossref
Bryant, L., Ferguson, A., & Spencer, E. (2016). Linguistic analysis of discourse in aphasia: A review of the literature. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 30(7), 489-518.
crossref
Cavanaugh, R. & Haley, K. L. (2020). Subjective communication difficulties in very mild aphasia. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 29(1S), 437-448.
crossref pmid
Doyle, P. J., Goda, A. J., & Spencer, K. A. (1995). The communicative informativeness and efficiency of connected discourse by adults with aphasia under structured and conversational sampling conditions. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 4(4), 130-134.
crossref
Edwards, S. (2005). Fluent aphasia. (1st ed.), (pp.1-245). New York (NY): Cambridge University Press.

Fergadiotis, G., Wright, H. H., & Capilouto, G. J. (2011). Productive vocabulary across discourse types. Aphasiology, 25(10), 1261-1278.
crossref pmid pmc
Hazamy, A. A. & Obermeyer, J. (2020). Evaluating informative content and global coherence in fluent and non‐fluent aphasia. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 55(1), 110-120.
crossref pdf
Heeschen, C. & Schegloff, E. A. (2003). Aphasic agrammatism as interactional artifact and achievement. Conversation and Brain Damage, 231-282.

Hema, N. & Shyamala, K. C. (2008). Study of discourse analysis in traumatic brain injury: Left hemisphere damage v/s right hemisphere damage. Students Research at All India Institute of Speech and Hearing, 3, 127-146.

Manning, M. & Franklin, S. (2016). Cognitive grammar and aphasic discourse. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 30(6), 417-432.
crossref
Marangolo, P., Fiori, V., Campana, S., Calpagnano, M. A., Razzano, C., Caltagirone, C., et al. (2014). Something to talk about: Enhancement of linguistic cohesion through tdCS in chronic non fluent aphasia. Neuropsychologia, 53, 246-256.
crossref pmid
McCullough, K. C., Lance, D., Beverly, B. L., McCullough, K. C., Lance, D., & Beverly, B. L. (2017). Comparison of structured and unstructured discourse tasks in persons with aphasia. Clinical Archives of Communication Disorders, 2(1), 23-29.
crossref pdf
McNeil, M., Doyle, P., Hula, W., Rubinsky, H., Fossett, T., & Matthews, C. (2004). Using resource allocation theory and dual‐task methods to increase the sensitivity of assessment in aphasia. Aphasiology, 18(5-7), 521-542.
crossref
Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bédirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53(4), 695-699.
crossref pmid
Nicholas, L. E. & Brookshire, R. H. (1993). A system for quantifying the informativeness and efficiency of the connected speech of adults with aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 36(2), 338-350.
crossref
Olness, G. S. & Ulatowska, H. K. (2011). Personal narratives in aphasia: Coherence in the context of use. Aphasiology, 25(11), 1393-1413.
crossref pmid pmc
Pritchard, M., Hilari, K., Cocks, N., & Dipper, L. (2017). Reviewing the quality of discourse information measures in aphasia. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 52(6), 689-732.
crossref pdf
Richardson, J. D. & Dalton, S. G. H. (2020). Main concepts for two picture description tasks: An addition to Richardson and Dalton, 2016. Aphasiology, 34(1), 119-136.
crossref pmid
Roberts, J. A. & Wertz, R. T. (1989). Comparison of spontaneous and elicited oral-expressive language in aphasia. Clinical Aphasiology, 18, 479-488.

Sekine, K., Rose, M. L., Foster, A. M., Attard, M. C., & Lanyon, L. E. (2013). Gesture production patterns in aphasic discourse: In-depth description and preliminary predictions. Aphasiology, 27(9), 1031-1049.
crossref
Shadden, B. B., Burnette, R. B., Eikenberry, B. R., & DiBrezzo, R. (1991). All discourse tasks are not created equal. Clinical Aphasiology, 20, 327-342.

Sherratt, S. (2007). Multi‐level discourse analysis: A feasible approach. Aphasiology, 21(3-4), 375-393.
crossref
Stark, B. C. (2019). A comparison of three discourse elicitation methods in aphasia and age-matched adults: Implications for language assessment and outcome. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 28(3), 1067-1083.
crossref pmid pmc
Stark, B. C., Alexander, J. M., Hittson, A., Doub, A., Igleheart, M., Streander, T., & et al.. (2023). Test-retest reliability of microlinguistic information derived from spoken discourse in persons with chronic aphasia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 66(7), 2316-2345.
crossref
Stemmer, B. (1999). Discourse studies in neurologically impaired populations: A quest for action. Brain and Language, 68(3), 402-418.
crossref pmid
Sweller, J., Ayres, P., Kalyuga, S., Sweller, J., Ayres, P., & Kalyuga, S. (2011). Intrinsic and extraneous cognitive load. Cognitive Load Theory, 57-69.
crossref
Vandenborre, D., Visch‐Brink, E., van Dun, K., Verhoeven, J., & Mariën, P. (2018). Oral and written picture description in individuals with aphasia. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 53(2), 294-307.
crossref pdf
Wright, H. H. (2011). Discourse in aphasia: An introduction to current research and future directions. Aphasiology, 25(11), 1283-1285.
crossref
Zhang, M., Geng, L., Yang, Y., & Ding, H. (2021). Cohesion in the discourse of people with post-stroke aphasia. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 35(1), 2-18.
crossref

APPENDICES

Appendix 2.

Discourse domains considered for the propostional and non-propostional aspects of speech

Aspect Rate Remarks
Narration
 Discourse structure
  Discourse forethought
  Organizational planning
 Communication intent
  Initiation of narration
  Asks for assistance
  Imagines events
 Coherence
  Global
  Local
 Topic management
  Introducing topic
  Topic shifts
  Topic changes
  Perseverations
  Minimal elaborations
  Topic elaboration
 Information adequacy
 Information content
 Message accuracy
 Temporal and causal relation
 Vocabulary specificity
 Linguistic fluency
 Speech style
 Intonation
Non-propositional
 Revision behavior
 Repair strategy
  Self-correction
  Repair through repetition
  Initiated correction
  Request for clarification
Total
Conversation
 Discourse structure
  Discourse forethought
  Organizational planning
 Communication intent
  Greeting and introduction
  Starts conversation
  Asks information
  Asks assistance
  Criticizing
  Imagination of events
  Understands blockers and advancers
 Coherence
  Global
  Local
 Topic management
  Introducing topic
  Topic shifts
  Topic changes
  Perseverations
  Responses with expand topic
  Minimal responses
  Minimal elaborations
  Topic elaboration
 Information adequacy
 Information content
 Message accuracy
 Vocabulary specificity
 Linguistic fluency
 Speech style
 Intonation
 Gaze efficiency
 Response time
Non-propositional
 Turn taking
  Initiation of turn
  Time to start a turn
  Contingency of turn
  Prosody
  Mode of conversation
  Listeners or speakers mode
 Revision behaviors
 Conversation repair
  Use of self-repair
  Use of revisions
  Use of initiated repair
Total
Picture description
 Discourse structure
  Discourse forethought
  Organizational Planning
 Communication Intent
  Initiation of narration
  Asks for assistance
  Criticizes events
  Imagines events
 Coherence
  Global
  Local
 Topic management
  Introducing topic
  Topic shifts
  Topic changes
  Perseverations
  Minimal elaborations
  Topic elaboration
 Information adequacy
 Information content
 Message accuracy
 Vocabulary specificity
 Linguistic fluency
 Speech style
 Intonation
 Response time
 Gist of information
Non-proportional
 Revision behaviors
 Repair strategy
  Self-correction
  Repair through repetition
  Initiated correction
  Request for clarification
Total
TOOLS
PDF Links  PDF Links
PubReader  PubReader
ePub Link  ePub Link
XML Download  XML Download
Full text via DOI  Full text via DOI
Download Citation  Download Citation
  Print
Share:      
METRICS
0
Crossref
0
Scopus
294
View
14
Download
Related article
Editorial Office
RN. 8602, Hallym University,
1 Hallymdaehak-gil, Chuncheon-si, Gangwon-do 24252, Korea
TEL: +82-70-8274-4268   FAX: +82-33-256-3420   E-mail: audiologykorea@gmail.com
About |  Browse Articles |  Current Issue |  For Authors
Copyright © Korean Academy of Audiology.                 Developed in M2PI